An Exploration of Language-Based Multiplicity in Fluent Bilinguals

  • Johanna Tiemann Private Practice

Abstract


This study used projective tests to explore the language-based shifts in aspects of personality observed in fluent bilinguals. The Rorschach Comprehensive System (RCS) and the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) were administered in Spanish and in English to 26 fluent Spanish/English bilinguals, and protocols were compared based on measures of affect, self-perception, cognitive complexity, and object relations. No significant differences were found between language protocols for most measures, nor did these measures relate to age of second language learning, relative use of English and Spanish, or participation in therapy. However, cognitive effort was significantly greater on English RCS protocols, and a strong correlation between level of acculturation and freedom of expression in English was found. Further exploration of the data revealed very important differences between most subjects’ Spanish and English protocols. Most notably, in 73% of the cases, RCS protocols differed so extensively by language that different key variable for cluster interpretation and diagnosis were indicated, and 42% of subjects’ modes of decision making (EB style) varied according to language. These findings are remarkable because these key variables are believed to measure constant, dominant and trait-like features of an individual’s psychology. The findings underscored the need for norms based on this population, and development of culturally-based constructs for interpretation of the RCS. 

References

Abu-Lughod, L., & Lutz, A. (Eds.) (1990). Language and the politics of emotion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Amati-Mehler, J., Argentieri, S., & Canestri, J. (1993). The babel of the unconscious: Mother tongue and foreign languages in the psychoanalytic dimension. Madison,
CT: International Universities Press.

Anooshian, L. J., & Hertel, P. T. (1994). Emotionality and free recall: language specificity in bilingual memory. Cognition and Emotion, 8(6), 503-514.

Antinucci-Mark, G. (1990). Speaking in tongues in the consulting room or the dialectic of
foreigners. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 6, 375-383.

Ardila, A. (1998). Bilingualism: A neglected and chaotic area. Aphasiology, 12(2),131-134.

Ben-Rafael, M. (2004). Language contact and attrition: The spoken French of Israeli Francophones. In M. S. Schmid, B. Kopke, M. Keijzer, & L. Weileman (Eds.), First language attrition: Interdisciplinary perpectives on methodological issues (pp. 165-187). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.

Bond, M. H., & Lai, T. (1986). Embarrassment and code-switching into a second language. Journal of Social Psychology, 126, 179-186.

Bromberg, P. M. (1996). Standing in the spaces: the multiplicity of self and the psychoanalytic relationship. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 32(4), 509-535.

Buxbaum, E. (1949). The role of a second language in the formation of ego and superego.
Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 18(3), 279-289.

Casas, J. M., & Vasquez, M. J. T. (1989). Counseling the Hispanic client: A theoretical and applied perspective. In P. B. Pedersen et al. (Eds.) Counseling Across Cultures (pp. 116-133). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.

Chefetz, R. A., & Bromberg, P. M. (2004). Talking with “Me” and “Not-Me”: A dialogue. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 40, 409-464.

Clauss, S. C. (1998). Language: The unspoken variable in psychotherapy practice. Psychotherapy, 35(2), 188-196.

Davies, J. (1999). Getting cold feet, defining “safe-enough” borders: Dissociation, multiplicity, and integration and the analyst’s experience. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 68, 184-208.

Del Castillo, J. (1970). The influence of language upon symptomotology in foreign-born patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 127, 242-244.

Dewaele, J.-M. (2009). Age effects on self-perceived communicative competence and language choice among adult multilinguals. EUROSLA Yearbook, 9(1), 245-268.

Dewaele, J.-M. (2004a). The emotional force of swearwords and taboo words in the speech of multilinguals. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 25(2), 204-222.

Dewaele, J.-M. (2004b). Blistering barnacles! What language do multilinguals swear in? Estudios de Sociolinguistica, 5(1), 83-105.

Dewaele, J.-M. (2004c). Perceived language dominance and language preference for emotional speech: The implications for attrition research. In M. S. Schmid et al. (Eds.), First language attribution: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues (pp. 81-104). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Benjamins.

Dewaele, J.-M., & Pavlenko, A. (2002). Emotional vocabulary in interlanguage. Language Learning, 52(2), 263-322.

Doi, T. (1973). The anatomy of independence. Tokyo, Japan: Kodansh International. Ervin, S.M. (1964). Language and T.A.T. content in bilinguals. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 68, 500-507.

Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1968). An analysis of the interaction of language, topic, and listener.

In J. Fishman (Ed.), Readings in the sociology of language (pp. 192-211). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.

Exner, J. E. (1980). But it’s only an inkblot. Journal of Personality Assessment, 44, 562-577.

Exner, J. E. (1991). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system. Interpretation: Vol. 2. (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Exner, J. E. (1993). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system. Basic foundations: Vol.1. (3rd ed.), New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Fuster, J., Sifre, S., Barriusi, I., Lobato, E., & Martinez, M. (1997). Comparación de una muestra de población normal valenciana con la muestra barcelonasa [Comparison of a sample of normal population from Barcelona with the sample from Barcelona]. Revista de la Sociedad Española del Rorschach y Metodos Proyectivos, 10, 58- 65.

Geertz, C. (1984). “From the native’s point of view”: On the nature of anthropological understanding. In R. Shweder, & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 137-157). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Goddard, C. (1991). Anger in the western desert: A case study in the cross-cultural semantic means of emotion. Man, 26, 265-279.

Greenson, R.R. (1950). The mother tongue and the mother. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 31(18), 18-23.

Grosjean, F. (1982). Life with two languages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Haller, N., & Exner, J. E. (1985). The reliability of Rorschach variables for inpatients presenting symptoms of depression and/or helplessness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 516-521.

Harris, C. L., Aycicegi, A., & Gleason, J. B. (2003). Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language. Applied Linguistics, 24(4), 561-579.

Heelas, P. (1983). Indigenous representations of emotions: the Chewong. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Oxford, 14(1), 87-103.

Hill, S. (2008). Language and intersubjectivity: Multiplicity in a bilingual treatment. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 18, 437-455.

Hoffman, E. (1989). Lost in translation: A life in a new language. New York, NY: Dutton.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Cultures consequences: International differences in work-related values. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Javier, R. A. (1995). Vicissitudes of autobiographical memories in a bilingual analysis. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 12(3), 429-438.

Kaplan, A. (1993). French lessons. Chicago, Il: The University of Chicago Press.

Kitron, D. (1992). Transference and countertransference implications conducted in a foreign language. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 56, 232-245.

Krapf, E. E. (1955). The choice of language in polyglot psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 24, 343-357.

Lijtmaer, R. M. (1999). Language shift and bilinguals: transference and countertransference implications. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 27(4), 611-623.

Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and their challenge to western theory. Chicago, Il: University of Chicago Press.

Lvovich, N. (1997). The multilingual self: An inquiry into language learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Koreneva, E. (2003). She-idiot: An autobiography. Moscow, Russia: ACT Astrel’. Malawista, K. L., & Drake, D. S. (2002). Conversion symptoms and psychoanalysis in a second language: some thoughts on the analysis of Mr. B. Psychoanalytic Social Work, 9A, 3-25.

Marcos, L. R., & Alpert, M. (1976). Strategies and risks in psychotherapy with bilingual patients: The phenomenon of language independence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 133(11), 1275-1278.

Marcos, L. R., Eisma, J. E., & Guimon, J. (1977). Bilingualism and sense of self. The American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 37, 285-290.

Marcos, L. R., & Urcuyo, L. (1979). Dynamic psychotherapy with the bilingual patient. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 33(3), 331-338.

Marcos, L. R. (1988). Understanding ethnicity in psychotherapy with Hispanics. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 48, 35-42.

Marin, G., & Gamba, R. J. (1996). A new measurement of acculturation for Hispanics: The bidimensional acculturation scale for Hispanics (BAS). Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 18(3), 297-316.

McMahill, C. (2001). Self-expression, gender, and community: A Japanese feminist English class. In A. Pavlenko, A. Blackledge, I. Piller, & M. Teutsch-Dwyer (Eds), Multilingualism, second language learning, and gender (pp. 307-344). Berlin, Germany: Mouton De Gruyter.

Morsbach, H., & Tyler, W. (1986). A Japanese emotion: Amae. In R. Harre (Ed.), The social construction of emotions (pp. 289-307). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Murray, H. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test
manual. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (1996). Narrating the self. Annual Review of Anthropology, 25, 19-43.

Panayiotou, A. (2004a). Bilingual emotions: The untranslatable self. Estudios de Sociolingüística, 5(1), 1-19.

Panayiotou, A. (2004b). Switching codes, switching code: Bilinguals’ emotional responses in English and Greek. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 25(2&3), 124-139.

Pavlenko, A. (1998). Second language learning by adults: Testimonies of bilingual writers. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 9(1), 3-19.

Pavlenko, A. (2002a). Emotions and the body in Russian and English. Pragmatics and Cognition, 10(1-2), 201-26.

Pavlenko, A. (2002b). Bilingualism and emotions. Multilingua, 21(1), 45-78.

Pavlenko, A. (2008). Emotion and emotion-laden words in the bilingual lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 147-164.

Perez Foster, R. (1992). Psychoanalysis and the bilingual patient: Some observations on the influence of language choice on the transference. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 9(1), 61-75.

Perez Foster, R. (1996). Assessing the psychodynamic function of language in the bilingual patient. In R. Perez Foster, M. Moskowitz, & R. A. Javier (Eds.), Reaching across the boundaries of culture and class (pp. 243-263). New Jersey, NJ: Jason Aronson.

Perex-Foster, R. (1998). Special issues in working with the bilingual person: The power of language in the clinical process: Assessing and treating the bilingual person. New Jersey, NJ: Jason Aronson.

Piller, I., & Takahashi, K. (2006). A passion for English: Desire and the language market. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), Bilingual minds: Emotional experiencing, expression, and representation (pp. 59-83). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

Rintell, E. (1984). But how did you feel about that? The learner’s perception of emotion in speech. Applied Linguistics, 5(3), 255-264.

Roberts, P. M. (1998). Reply: Bilingual aphasia: Some answers and more questions. Aphasiology, 12(2), 141-146.

Rosado, J. (1980). Important psychocultural factors in the delivery of mental health services to lower-class Puerto Rican clients: A review of recent studies. Journal of Community Psychology, 8(2), 215-226.

Rosaldo, M. (1984). Toward an anthropology of self and feeling. In R. Shweder, & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 137-157). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Rozensky, R. H., & Gomez, M. Y. (1983). Language switching in psychotherapy with bilinguals: two problems, tow models, and case examples. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 20(2), 152-160.

Rubin, D.C., & Friendly, M. (1986). Predicting which words get recalled: Measures of free recall, availability, goodness, emotionality, and pronunciability for 925 nouns. Memory and Cognition, 14, 79-94.

Russell, J. (1991). Culture and categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 426-450.

Sante, L. (1996, May 12). Living in tongues. The New York Times Magazine, 31-34.

Shaffer, T. W., Erdberg, P., & Haroian, J. (1999). Current nonpatient data for the Rorschach, WAIS-R, and MMPI-2. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73(2), 305-316.

Stern, D. (2003). Unformulated experience. New York, NY: Routledge.

Stern, D. (2004). The eye sees itself: Dissociation, enactment, and the achievement of conflict. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 40, 197-237.

Torres, W.J. (1983). Puerto Rican and Anglo conceptions of appropriate mental health services. In K.E. Davis, & R. Bergner (Eds.), Advances in descriptive psychology (pp. 147-170), Greenwich, CT: JAI Press Inc.

Vinet, E. V. (2000). The Rorschach Comprehensive System in Iberoamerica. In R. Dana (Ed.), Handbook of cross-cultural and multicultural personality assessment (pp. 345-365). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K. Kerber, K., & Goodrich, S. (1985). Object relations and social cognition TAT scoring manual. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.

Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Talking about emotions: Semantics, culture, and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 285-319.
Published
01-Dec-2012
How to Cite
Tiemann, J. (2012). An Exploration of Language-Based Multiplicity in Fluent Bilinguals. Language and Psychoanalysis, 1(1), 42-67. https://doi.org/10.7565/landp.2012.0004
Section
Original Articles